Saturday, 29 November 2014

Critique of Environmental Orientation

Recently, researchers have shown an increased interest in Environmental Education (EE) and what it can do for children in the education sectors. This critique will focus on a journal by Larson, Castleberry and Green (2010) the “Effects of an Environmental Education Program on the Environmental Orientation of Children from Different Gender, Age and Ethnic Groups”. The main objectives during this study were to look at baseline differences in the three key points made in the title: gender, age and ethnic background, the study was sponsored by the State Botanical Garden of Georgia who wanted to see children’s eco-affinity, eco-awareness and their environmental knowledge. During this study 133 children aged between six to thirteen years old took part. The researchers also wanted to examine the effects of them taking part in a one week EE program at a summer camp. The responses from the program would be compared to a control group who were a group of students who were enrolled in an after school program. By looking at these areas the researchers believed that it would give them effective strategies for improving EE programs.

Background literature on the three points made in the title suggested that most studies done on environmental orientation are usually focused on adults and that research had found that women were more inclined to support protection of nature and express their concerns about environmental issues than men (Milfont & Duckitt, 2004). However, other research carried out on the area of gender suggests that there is a “gender gap” that advocates women are less knowledgeable about the environment (Coyle, 2005). When reading the information given about age and environmental orientations Whittaker, Sagura and Bowler (2005) said that willingness to self-identify as environmentalists declined when becoming young adults. Background reading on ethnicity suggested that African Americans and Hispanic children had fewer opportunities to access natural resources, parks ect (Bullard, 2006).

From the results there showed no significant difference in gender across the children’s eco-affinity, eco-awareness and their environmental knowledge scores. In age of children ages 9 or below and 10 and above showed significant difference in eco-affinity with it being lower in older children. Eco-awareness was not observed as being significantly different in the age groups and the environmental knowledge did not show statically evidence for being significantly different, however the mean scores were slightly higher in the older age group. Ethnicity showed significant difference in eco-awareness and knowledge. With African American children scoring considerably lower in both fields. However, eco-affinity was virtually the same in both ethnic backgrounds so showed no significant difference.

From these results you’re able to come to the conclusion that environmental orientation can be affected by a multiple of factors. Age suggested that the older children’s knowledge about the environment was better but their drive to liking and understanding something (eco-affinity) was lower than the younger children which concur with Whittaker etal. (2005) statement. The main factor that showed the biggest significance was ethnicity. During the short interviews one African American child says,

“I don’t go outside at my house because it’s like a bad, you know, environment to
go out in. So my mom takes me to the park and we go play basketball, go on the
swings, slide down the slide, and sometimes we walk.”

One of the main points you should take away from this journal is that all children should be given the same opportunity to experience the outdoors. However, they are not, so environmental orientation will be most different in ethnicity than the other fields.

Most of the children enjoyed the activities that had some form of physical activity, they enjoyed learning about plants and animals but what the researchers really got from the interviews was that it was “fun!”

It is said that out of school activities can spark an interest in nature and have a deep impact on effective development (Uitto, Juuti, Lavonen & Meisalo, 2006). This suggests that non-formal EE programs can play and important role in connecting children with nature and making more environmental literate people for the future.

This reading is a good source that informs the reader about the difference aspects that affect an individual’s environmental orientation. It also informs you about how informal EE sessions can benefit children’s developments and help them in later life be more environmentally literate people. The paper gets first hand data from the children and what they think and know of the environment. Limitations of the study are the amount of participants and that many had to drop out due to a number of reasons leaving them with 133, resulting in maybe not being as accurate in the display of children’s environmental orientation over a general population.


Overall the paper has its positives and negatives but is an enjoyable informative read.

References
Bullard, R. D. (2006). Anatomy of environmental racism and the environmental justice
movement. In Bullard, R. D., Confronting environmental racism: Voices from
the grassroots. Boston, MA: South End Press.

Coyle, K. (2005). Environmental literacy in America: What ten years of NEETF/Roper
research and related studies say about environmental literacy in the U.S. Washington,
D.C.: The National Environmental Education & Training Foundation.

Milfont, T.L. & Duckitt, J. (2004). The structure of environmental attitudes: A first- and
second-order confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24,
289-303.

Uitto, A. Juuti, K. Lavonen, J. & Meisalo, V. (2006). Students’ interest in biology and
their out-of-school experiences. Journal of Biological Education, 40, 124-129.

Whittaker, M. Segura, G. M. & Bowler, S. (2005). Racial/ethnic group attitudes
toward environmental protection in California: Is “environmentalism” still a white
phenomenon? Political Research Quarterly, 58, 435-447.